giovedì 26 gennaio 2017

Social Media and Fake News in the 2016 Election Hunt Allcott

Notebook per
Social Media and Fake News in the 2016 Election
riccardo-mariani@libero.it
Citation (APA): riccardo-mariani@libero.it. (2017). Social Media and Fake News in the 2016 Election [Kindle Android version]. Retrieved from Amazon.com

Parte introduttiva
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 2
Social Media and Fake News in the 2016 Election Hunt Allcott, New York University and NBER ∗ Matthew Gentzkow,
Nota - Posizione 3
t
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 3
Abstract
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 4
Drawing on audience data, archives of fact-checking websites,
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 5
(i) social media was an important but not dominant source
Nota - Posizione 5
DOMINANZA
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 6
(ii) of the known false news stories that appeared in the three months before the election, those favoring Trump were shared a total of 30 million times on Facebook, while those favoring Clinton were shared eight million times;
Nota - Posizione 7
x CONDIVISIOONE
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 7
(iii) the average American saw and remembered 0.92 pro-Trump fake news stories and 0.23 pro-Clinton fake news stories, with just over half of those who recalled seeing fake news stories believing them;
Nota - Posizione 9
x CHI SI RICORDA E CHI HA CREDUTO
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 9
(iv) for fake news to have changed the outcome of the election, a single fake article would need to have had the same persuasive effect as 36 television campaign ads.
Nota - Posizione 10
x CFR CON CAMPAGNE TELEVISIVE
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 13
1 Introduction
Nota - Posizione 13
t
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 14
In the 19th century, cheap newsprint and better presses allowed partisan newspapers to dramatically expand their reach. Many have argued that the effectiveness of the press as a check on power was significantly compromised as a result (Kaplan 2002).
Nota - Posizione 14
x GIORNALI MILITANTI
Nota - Posizione 15
x FINE STAMPA CANE DA GUARDIA
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 15
In the 20th century, as radio and then television became the dominant platforms, observers worried they would reduce substantive policy debates
Nota - Posizione 16
x RADIO E TV
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 17
“telegenic” candidates
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 18
(Lang and Lang 2002; Bagdikian 1983).
Nota - Posizione 18
su
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 19
excess diversity of viewpoints would make it easier for like-minded citizens to form “echo chambers” or “filter bubbles”
Nota - Posizione 20
x PREOCCUPAZ SM
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 20
(Sunstein 2001a, 2001b, 2009; Pariser 2011).
Nota - Posizione 20
su
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 23
user with no track record or reputation can in some cases reach as many readers as Fox News,
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 24
impact of false or misleading information
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 25
Recent evidence shows that: (i) 62 percent of U.S. adults get news on social media (Pew 2016a); (ii) the most popular fake news stories were more widely shared on Facebook than the most popular mainstream news stories (Silverman 2016); (iii) many people who see fake news stories report that they believe them (Silverman and Singer-Vine 2016); and (iv) the most discussed fake news stories tended to favor Donald Trump over Hillary Clinton (Silverman 2016).
Nota - Posizione 28
x ALCUNI FATTI
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 28
commentators have suggested that Donald Trump would not have been elected
Nota - Posizione 28
INFLUENZA
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 36
quantifying the importance of social media
Nota - Posizione 36
PASSO 1
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 36
Previous published work, new web browsing data, and a 1200-person post-election online survey we conducted for this paper during the week of November 28, 2016 are consistent in suggesting that social media was an important but not dominant source of information.
Nota - Posizione 38
x FONTI E CONC
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 38
14 percent
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 39
“most important”
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 39
Second, we collect a database of election stories that have been categorized as fake by leading fact checking websites
Nota - Posizione 40
x COLLEZIONE BUFALE
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 40
For each story, we record the number of times it has been shared on social media, and whether its assertion favored Hilary Clinton or Donald Trump. We confirm that fake news was both widely shared and tilted in favor of Donald Trump,
Nota - Posizione 41
c CONDIVISIONI E FAVORITISMI
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 42
30 million times,
Nota - Posizione 42
PRO TRUMP
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 43
7.6 million times.
Nota - Posizione 43
x PRO CLINTON
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 43
we use the results of our new survey to estimate the share of Americans who saw and believed
Nota - Posizione 44
x VEDERE E CREDERE
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 44
asking for each: (i) “Do you recall seeing this reported or discussed prior to the election?” and (ii) “At the time of the election, would your best guess have been that this statement was true?”
Nota - Posizione 45
c
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 46
placebo fake news headlines
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 48
about 15 percent of U.S. adults report that they recall seeing the average fake news headline. About 8 percent report seeing and believing it.
Nota - Posizione 49
c
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 49
However, these numbers are statistically identical for our placebo headlines, suggesting that the raw responses could overstate true exposure by an order of magnitude.
Nota - Posizione 50
x PLACEBO SIMILE. OVERSTATE
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 50
Using the difference between actual and placebo
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 51
we estimate that 1.2 percent of people recall seeing the average story.
Nota - Posizione 51
x 1,2
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 52
our point estimate suggests that the average voting-age American saw and remembered about 0.92 pro-Trump fake stories and 0.23 pro-Clinton fake stories
Nota - Posizione 53
c
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 55
voter persuasion model
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 56
DellaVigna and Kaplan (2007).
Nota - Posizione 56
SU E GIÙ
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 56
We assume that each fake news story someone sees and remembers has a fixed probability f of inducing her to vote for the candidate the story favors,
Nota - Posizione 57
x CAMBIO VOTO
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 59
Rather than estimate the persuasion rate, we benchmark it against recent estimates of the persuasive effects of television campaign advertisements from Spenkuch and Toniatti (2016).
Nota - Posizione 61
x PARAGONE CON LE CAMPAGNE TV
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 61
We estimate that in order for fake news to have changed the election result, the average fake story would need to have f ≈ 0.0073, making it about as persuasive as 36 television campaign ads.
Nota - Posizione 62
x CONCLUSIONI
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 69
our calibrated election model relies on a number of other strong assumptions, including a persuasion rate of fake news that is the same for all stories and voters and that cumulates linearly.
Nota - Posizione 70
x L ASSUNTO FORTE. FORZE NDECISI COME CONVONTI
Evidenzia (giallo) - Posizione 385
7 Conclusion
Nota - Posizione 385
t